Spatial incompatibility training can prevent the occurrence of the enhanced Simon effect in elderly
Palabras clave:
Ageing, Inhibitory control, Spatial perception, Transfer of learningResumen
The Simon effect is typically larger for older people than for young adults, maybe due to age-related decline in inhibitory capabilities. 32 right-handed aged people (5 male, 27 female; mean of 67,1 years ± 5,5) participated in this study. In screening were used the Mini-Mental State Examination and the Center for Epidemiological Study Depression Scale. Then, half of the participants practiced the spatial incompatibility task before the Simon task and the other group was only tested on the Simon task. The analysis of variance considering practice (incompatible, no-practice group) and correspondence (corresponding, non-corresponding) revealed a two-way interaction (F = 7.07; p = 0.012; ηp2 = 0.191) showing that the Simon effect was eliminated in the incompatible practice group, but remained intact in the group with no previous practice. These results indicated that cognitive processes required to the transfer of learning are preserved in elderly, being potentially useful to influence spatial inhibitory capabilities.
Descargas
Citas
Aisenberg, D., Sapir, A., Close, A., Henik, A., & d’Avossa, G. (2018). Right anterior cerebellum BOLD responses reflect age related changes in Simon task sequential effects. Neuropsychologia, 109, 155-164. https://doi.org/10.1093/ geronb/gbaa184
Ambrosecchia, M., Marino, B. F., Gawryszewski, L. G., & Riggio, L. (2015). Spatial stimulus-response compatibility and affordance effects are not ruled by the same mechanisms. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 283. https://doi. org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00283
Bae, G. Y., Cho, Y. S., & Proctor, R. W. (2009). Transfer of orthogonal stimulus-response mappings to an orthogonal Simon task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(4), 746-765. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210802303883
Batistoni, S. S. T., Néri, A. L., & Cupertino, A. P. (2010). Validade e confiabilidade da versão Brasileira da Center for Epidemiological Scale-Depression (CES-D) em idosos Brasileiros. Psico-USF, 15(1), 13-22. https://doi.org/10.1590/ S1413-82712010000100003
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290
Buchanan, J. A., Johnson, E., Kennedy, J., Stypulkowski, K., & Jensen, N. (2019). The effects of cognitive training program for cognitively impaired older adults: a pilot randomized control trial. International Journal of Aging Research, 2(2), 30-30. https://doi.org/10.28933/ijoar-2019-03-0705
Butler, M., McCreedy, E., Nelson, V. A., Desai, P., Ratner, E., Fink, H. A., Hemmy, L. S., McCarten, J. R., Barclay, T. R., Brasure, M., Davila, H., & Kane, R. L. (2018). Does cognitive training prevent cognitive decline? A systematic review. Annals of Internal Medicine, 168(1), 63-68. https://doi.org/10.7326/M17-1531
Cabeza, R., Nyberg, L., & Park, D. C. (2016). Cognitive neuroscience of aging: linking cognitive and cerebral aging. Oxford University Press.
Cespón, J., Hommel, B., Korsch, M., & Galashan, D. (2020). The neurocognitive underpinnings of the Simon effect: an integrative review of current research. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 20, 1-40. https://doi. org/10.3758/s13415-020-00836-y
Cohen, J. (2013). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Routledge.
Conde, E. F. Q., Fraga Filho, R. S., Lameira, A. P., Riggio, L., & Gawryszewski, L. G. (2017). Stimulus-response dimensional overlap elicits a decreasing Simon effect along the vertical dimension. Estudos de Psicologia, 22(4), 358-365. http:// dx.doi.org/10.22491/1678-4669.20170037
Conde, E. F. Q., Fraga-Filho, R. S., Lameira, A. P., Mograbi, D. C., Riggio, L., & Gawryszewski, L. G. (2015). Influence of short incompatible practice on the Simon effect: transfer along the vertical dimension and across vertical and horizontal dimensions. Experimental Brain Research, 233(11), 3313-3321. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4399-1
Creekmur, B., & Vu, K. P. (2011). Effect of prior practice on the stimulus-response compatibility effect in a mixed mapping environment. The American Journal of Psychology, 125(3), 335-349. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.125.3.0335
D’Ascenzo, S., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (2021). Practice effects vs. transfer effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 85, 1955-1969. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01386-1
Espírito-Santo, H., Pires, C. F., Garcia, I. Q., Daniel, F., Silva, A. G. D., & Fazio, R. L. (2017). Preliminary validation of the Portuguese Edinburgh Handedness Inventory in an adult sample. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 24(3), 275-287.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191. https://doi.org/10.3758/ BF03193146
Fjell, A. M., Westlye, L. T., Amlien, I., Espeseth, T., Reinvang, I., Raz, N., Agartz, I., Salat, D. H., Greve, D. N., Fischl, B., Dale, A. M., & Walhovd, K. B. (2009). High consistency of regional cortical thinning in aging across multiple samples. Cerebral Cortex, 19(9), 2001-2012. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn232
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189-198. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022- 3956(75)90026-6
Fuster, J. M. (2009). Cortex and memory: emergence of a new paradigm. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(11), 2047-2072. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21280
Grabbe, J. W., & Allen, P. A. (2012). Cross-task compatibility and age-related dual-task performance. Experimental Aging Research, 38(5), 469-487. https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2012.726154
Grady, C. (2012). The cognitive neuroscience of ageing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(7), 491-505. https://doi. org/10.1038/nrn3256
Hill, N. T., Mowszowski, L., Naismith, S. L., Chadwick, V. L., Valenzuela, M., & Lampit, A. (2016). Computerized cognitive training in older adults with mild cognitive impairment or dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 174(4), 329-340. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16030360
Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 849-878.
Hommel, B. (2011). The Simon effect as tool and heuristic. Acta Psychologica, 136(2), 189-202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. actpsy.2010.04.011
Hommel, B., & Lippa, Y. (1995). SR compatibility effects due to context-dependent spatial stimulus coding. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2(3), 370-374.
Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. L. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68(1), 1-17. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210974
Iani, C., Rubichi, S., Gherri, E., & Nicoletti, R. (2009). Co-occurrence of sequential and practice effects in the Simon task: Evidence for two independent mechanisms affecting response selection. Memory & Cognition, 37(3), 358-367. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.37.3.358
Ivanoff, J. (2003). On spatial response code activation in a Simon task. Acta Psychologica, 112(2), 157-179. https://doi. org/10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00081-1
Kattenstroth, J. C., Kolankowska, I., Kalisch, T., & Dinse, H. R. (2010). Superior sensory, motor, and cognitive performance in elderly individuals with multi-year dancing activities. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 2(31), 1-9. https://doi. org/10.3389/fnagi.2010.00031
Kawai, N., Kubo-Kawai, N., Kubo, K., Terazawa, T., & Masataka, N. (2012). Distinct aging effects for two types of inhibition in older adults: a near-infrared spectroscopy study on the Simon task and the flanker task. Neuroreport, 23(14), 819-824. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283578032
Krampe, R. T. (2002). Aging, expertise and fine motor movement. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(7), 769-776. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00064-7
Kubo-Kawai, N., & Kawai, N. (2010). Elimination of the enhanced Simon effect for older adults in a three-choice situation: ageing and the Simon effect in a go/no-go Simon task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(3), 452- 64. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210902990829
Liu, X., Banich, M. T., Jacobson, B. L., & Tanabe, J. L. (2004). Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial Stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI. Neuroimage, 22(3), 1097-1106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.02.033
Luo, C., & Proctor, R. W. (2019). How different direct association routes influence the indirect route in the same Simon-like task. Psychological Research, 83(8), 1733-1748. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1024-5
Lupien, S. J., & Wan, N. (2004). Successful ageing: from cell to self. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 359(1449), 1413-1426. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2004.1516
MacPherson, S. E., Phillips, L. H., & Della Sala, S. (2002). Age, executive function and social decision making: a dorsolateral prefrontal theory of cognitive aging. Psychology and Aging, 17(4), 598. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.598
Marini, M., Iani, C., Nicoletti, R., & Rubichi, S. (2011). Between-task transfer of learning from spatial compatibility to a color Stroop task. Experimental Psychology, 58(6), 473-479. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000115
McLaughlin, S. J., Jette, A. M., & Connell, C. M. (2012). An examination of healthy aging across a conceptual continuum: Prevalence estimates, demographic patterns, and validity. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biomedical Sciences and Medical Sciences, 67(7), 783-789. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glr234
Melara, R. D., Wang, H., Vu, K. P. L., & Proctor, R. W. (2008). Attentional origins of the Simon effect: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Brain Research, 1215, 147-159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2008.03.026
Melo, D. M. D., & Barbosa, A. J. G. (2015). Use of the Mini-Mental State Examination in research on the elderly in Brazil: a systematic review. Ciência & Saúde Coletiva, 20(12), 3865-3876. https://doi.org/10.1590/1413- 812320152012.06032015
Melo, D. M. D., Barbosa, A. J. G., Castro, N. R. D., & Neri, A. L. (2020). Mini-Mental State Examination in Brazil: an item response theory analysis. Paidéia, 30, e3014. https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-4327e3014
Miles, J. D., & Vu, K. P. L. (2021). Individual Response-Effect Congruencies Modulate Subsequent Stimulus - Response Compatibility Effects. The American Journal of Psychology, 134(1), 31-43. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.134.1.0031
Millner, A. J., Jaroszewski, A. C., Chamarthi, H., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2012). Behavioral and electrophysiological correlates of training-induced cognitive control improvements. NeuroImage, 63(2), 742-753. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. neuroimage.2012.07.032
Morrison, J. H., & Baxter, M. G. (2012). The ageing cortical synapse: hallmarks and implications for cognitive decline. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(4), 240. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3200
Nagy, B., Czigler, I., File, D., & Gaál, Z. A. (2020). Can irrelevant but salient visual cues compensate for the age-related decline in cognitive conflict resolution? An ERP study. Plos One, 15(5), e0233496. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal. pone.0233496
Nissim, N. R., O’Shea, A. M., Bryant, V., Porges, E. C., Cohen, R., & Woods, A. J. (2017). Frontal structural neural correlates of working memory performance in older adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 8, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.3389/ fnagi.2016.00328
Nyberg, L., & Pudas, S. (2019). Successful memory aging. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 219-243. https://doi. org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103052
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburg Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97-113. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
Opie, G. M., & Semmler, J. G. (2020). Characterising the influence of cerebellum on the neuroplastic modulation of intracortical motor circuits. Plos One, 15(7), e0236005. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236005
Ottoboni, G., Iani, C., Tessari, A., & Rubichi, S. (2013). Modulation of the affordance effect through transfer of learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(12), 2295-2302. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1080/174702 18.2013.863370
Park, D. C., Polk, T. A., Mikels, J. A., Taylor, S. F., & Marshuetz, C. (2001). Cerebral aging: integration of brain and behavioral models of cognitive function. Dialogues Clinical Neuroscience, 3(3), 151-65. https://doi.org/10.31887/ dcns.2001.3.3/dcpark
Persson, J., Nyberg, L., Lind, J., Larsson, A., Nilsson, L. G., Ingvar, M., & Buckner, R. L. (2006). Structure - function correlates of cognitive decline in aging. Cerebral Cortex, 16(7), 907-915. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhj036
Peterson, B. S., Kane, M. J., Alexander, G. M., Lacadie, C., Skudlarski, P., Leung, H. C., May, J., & Gore, J. C. (2002). An event-related functional MRI study comparing interference effects in the Simon and Stroop tasks. Cognitive Brain Research, 13(3), 427-440. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00054-x
Pick, D. F., & Proctor, R. W. (1999). Age differences in the effects of irrelevant location information. In M. Scerbo & M. W. Mouloua (Eds.), Automation technology and human performance (pp. 258-261). Erlbaum.
Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. L. (2006). Stimulus-response compatibility principles: data, theory, and applications. Taylor and Francis. https://doi.org/10.1201/9780203022795
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385-401. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F014662167700100306
Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., & Sheliga, B.M. (1994). Space and selective attention. In C. Umiltà & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance (pp. 231-265). MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00240962
Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., & Umiltá, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia, 25(1), 31-40. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028- 3932(87)90041-8
Scrivano, R. M., & Kieffaber, P. D. (2021). Behavioral and electrophysiological correlates of Simon and flanker conflict interference in younger and older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 29(2), 1-31. https://doi.org/10.1 080/13825585.2021.1874278
Soetens E., Maetens K., & Zeischka, P. (2010). Practice-induced and sequential modulations of the Simon effect. Atten Percept Psychophys, 72(4), 895-911. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00240962
Spierer, L., Chavan, C. F., & Manuel, A. L. (2013). Training induced behavioral and brain plasticity in inhibitory control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 427. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00427
Stoet, G. (2017). Sex differences in the Simon task help to interpret sex differences in selective attention. Psychological Research, 81(3), 571-581. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0763-4
Tagliabue, M., Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (2002). Cross-modal re-mapping influences the Simon effect. Memory and Cognition, 30, 18-23. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195261
Tagliabue, M., Zorzi, M., Umiltà, C., & Bassignani, F. (2000). The role of long-term-memory and short-term-memory links in the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(2), 648-670. https:// psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0096-1523.26.2.648
Van der Lubbe, R. H., & Verleger, R. (2002). Aging and the Simon task. Psychophysiology, 39(01), 100-110. https://doi. org/10.1111/1469-8986.3910100
Verghese, A., Mattingley, J. B., Palmer, P. E., & Dux, P. E. (2018). From eyes to hands: Transfer of learning in the Simon task across motor effectors. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 80(1), 193-210. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.3758/ s13414-017-1427-1
Vermeij, A., Claassen, J. A., Dautzenberg, P. L., & Kessels, R. P. (2016). Transfer and maintenance effects of online working-memory training in normal ageing and mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 26(5-6), 783-809. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2015.1048694
Vu, K. P. L. (2007). Influences on the Simon effect of prior practice with spatially incompatible mappings: transfer within and between horizontal and vertical dimensions. Memory & Cognition, 35(6), 1463-1471. https://doi.org/10.3758/ BF03193616
Vu, K. P. L., & Proctor, R. W. (2008). Age differences in response selection for pure and mixed stimulus-response mappings and tasks. Acta Psychologica, 129(1), 49-60. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.actpsy.2008.04.006
Wang, L., & Weekes, B. (2014). Neural correlates of the Simon effect modulated by practice with spatial mapping. Neuropsychologia, 63, 72-84. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.019
Wang, M., Gamo, N. J., Yang, Y., Jin, L. E., Wang, X. J., Laubach, M., Mazer, J. A., Lee, D., & Arnsten, A. F. (2011). Neuronal basis of age-related working memory decline. Nature, 476(7359), e210. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature10243
Ward, E., Berry, C., & Shanks, D. (2013). Age effects on explicit and implicit memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, e639. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00639
Wollesen, B., Wildbredt, A., van Schooten, K. S., Lim, M. L., & Delbaere, K. (2020). The effects of cognitive-motor training interventions on executive functions in older people: a systematic review and meta-analysis. European Review of Aging and Physical Activity, 17(1), 1-22. https://doi.org/10.1186/s11556-020-00240-y
Yamaguchi, M., & Proctor, R. W. (2009). Transfer of learning in choice reactions: contributions of specific and general components of manual responses. Acta Psychologica, 130(1), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.09.008
Zhong, Q., Xiong, A., Vu, K. P. L., & Proctor, R. W. (2018). Vertically arrayed stimuli and responses: transfer of incompatible spatial mapping to Simon task occurs regardless of response-device orientation. Experimental Brain Research, 236(1), 175-185. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-020-01998-0
Descargas
Publicado
Cómo citar
Número
Sección
Licencia
Derechos de autor 2023 Taciana Elaine de Moura DIAS, Fabíola Freire Lauria CAVALCANTI, Walter MACHADO-PINHEIRO, André dos Santos COSTA, Erick Francisco Quintas CONDE
Esta obra está bajo una licencia internacional Creative Commons Atribución 4.0.