Conspiracy theories: Meanings in the Brazilian context

Authors

Keywords:

Beliefs, Meaning, Theories

Abstract

The aim of the study was to understand the meanings attributed to conspiracy theories in a Brazilian sample. Participants were 383 undergraduate students (54.6% females; Mage = 21.9) from a public institution in João Pessoa, Paraíba. They answered demographic questions and two open-ended questions (What do you understand by conspiracy theories?; What comes to your mind when you think of conspiracy theories?). Based on the descending hierarchical classification analysis, five classes were identified: (1) theories without scientific foundation, (2) manipulation of secret groups, (3) explanation of social reality, (4) contestation of social facts, and (5) information control. In general, the meanings attributed to conspiracy theories were associated with the explanation of events in social reality and existence of secret groups that control important events. These findings were discussed in light of studies on conspiracy theories in Psychology, and they revealed congruence with the conception that was developed for this construct.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

References

Aaronovitch, D., & Langton, J. (2010). Voodoo histories: The role of the conspiracy theory in shaping modern history. New York: Riverhead Books.

Barron, D., Morgan, K., Towell, T., Altemeyer, B., & Swami, V. (2014). Associations between schizotypy and belief in conspiracist ideation. Personality and Individual Differences, 70, 156-159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.04

Bessi, A., Coletto, M., Davidescu, G. A., Scala, A., Caldarelli, G., & Quattrociocchi, W. (2015). Science vs conspiracy: Collective narratives in the age of misinformation. PLoS One, 10(2), 1-17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.011809

Bogart, L. M., & Thorburn, S. (2006). Relationship of African Americans’ sociodemographic characteristics to belief in conspiracies about HIV/AIDS and birth control. Journal of the National Medical Association, 98(7), 1144-1150.

Brotherton, R., & Eser, S. (2015). Bored to fears: Boredom proneness, paranoia, and conspiracy theories. Personality and Individual Differences, 80, 1-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.01

Byford, J. (2011). Conspiracy theories: A critical introduction. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

Byford, J. (2014). Beyond belief: The social psychology of conspiracy theories and the study of ideology. In C. Antaki & S. Condor (Eds.), Rhetoric, ideology and social psychology: Essays in honour of Michael Billig (pp. 83-94). London:

Routledge.

Camargo, B. V. (2005). ALCESTE: um programa informático de análise quantitativa de dados textuais. Perspectivas Teórico-Metodológicas em Representações Sociais, 1(3), 511-539.

Camargo, B. V., & Justo, A. M. (2013). IRAMUTEQ: um software gratuito para análise de dados textuais. Temas em Psicologia, 21(2), 513-518. http://dx.doi.org/10.9788/TP2013.2-16

Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2008). The hidden impact of conspiracy theories: Perceived and actual influence of theories surrounding the death of Princess Diana. The Journal of Social Psychology, 148(2), 210-222..

Drinkwater, K., Dagnall, N., & Parker, A. (2012). Reality testing, conspiracy theories, and paranormal beliefs. The Journal of Parapsychology, 76(1), 57-68.

Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 15(4), 731-742. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3791630

Goertzel, T. (2010). Conspiracy theories in science. EMBO Reports, 11(7), 493-499. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/embor.2010.8

Gouveia, V. V. (2013). Teoria funcionalista dos valores humanos: fundamentos, aplicações e perspectivas. São Paulo: Caso do Psicólogo.

Hofstadter, R. (1966). The paranoid style in American politics. In R. Hofstader (Ed.), The paranoid style in American politics and other essays (pp. 3-40). New York: Knopf.

Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (2014a). The effects of anti-vaccine conspiracy theories on vaccination intentions. PLoS One, 9(2), 898-906. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0089177

Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (2014b). The social consequences of conspiracism: Exposure to conspiracy theories decreases intentions to engage in politics and to reduce one’s carbon footprint. British Journal of Psychology, 105(1), 35-56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12018

Jolley, D., Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2017). Blaming a few bad apples to save a threatened barrel: The systemjustifying function of conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 39(2), 465-478. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/pops.12404

Knight, P. (2008). Outrageous conspiracy theories: Popular and official responses to 9/11 in Germany and the United States. New German Critique, 35(1), 165-193. http://dx.doi.org/10.1215/0094033X-2007-024

Kruglanski, A. W. (1987). Blame-placing schemata and attributional research. In C. F. Graumann & S. Moscovici (Eds.), Changing conceptions of conspiracy (pp. 219-229). New York: Springer.

Leman, P. J., & Cinnirella, M. (2013). Beliefs in conspiracy theories and the need for cognitive closure. Frontiers in Psychology, 378(4), 1-10. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00378

Lobato, E., Mendoza, J., Sims, V., & Chin, M. (2014). Examining the relationship between conspiracy theories, paranormal beliefs, and pseudoscience acceptance among a university population. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(5), 617-625. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.3042

Moulding, R., Nix-Carnell, S., Schnabel, A., Nedeljkovic, M., Burnside, E. E., Lentini, A. F., & Mehzabin, N. (2016). Better the devil you know than a world you don’t? Intolerance of uncertainty and worldview explanations for belief in conspiracy theories. Personality and Individual Differences, 98, 345-354. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.060

Nicolas, L. (2017). As teorias da conspiração como espelho do século: entre a retórica, a sociologia e a história das ideias. Revista Eletrônica de Estudos Integrados em Discurso e Argumentação, 12, 255-279. Recuperado em outubro 23, 2017, de http://periodicos.uesc.br/index.php/eidea/article/viewFile/1325/1087

Oliver, J. E., & Wood, T. J. (2014). Conspiracy theories and the paranoid style(s) of mass opinion. American Journal of Political Science, 58(4), 952-966. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12084

R Development Core Team (2012). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Ratinaud, P. (2009). IRAMUTEQ: Interface de R pour les analyses multidimensionnelles de textes et de questionnaires [Computer software]. Retrieved October 23, 2017, from http://www. iramuteq. org

Ratinaud, P., & Marchand, P. (2012). Application de la méthode ALCESTE à de “gros” corpus et stabilité des “mondes lexicaux”: Analyse du “Cable-Gate” avec IraMuTeQ. In Actes des 11eme Journées internationales d’Analyse Statistique des Données Textuelles (pp. 835-844). Récupéré dans Octobre 23, 2017, from http://lexicometrica.univparis3.fr/jadt/jadt2012/Communications/Ratinaud,%20Pierre%20et%20al.%20%20Application%20de%20la%20methode%20Alceste.pdf

Sapountzis, A., & Condor, S. (2013). Conspiracy accounts as intergroup theories: Challenging dominant understandings of social power and political legitimacy. Political Psychology, 34(5), 731-752. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/pops.1201

Swami, V., & Coles, R. (2010). The truth is out there: Belief in conspiracy theories. The Psychologist, 23(7), 560-563.

Swami, V., & Furnham, A. (2012). Examining conspiracist beliefs about the disappearance of Amelia Earhart. The Journal of General Psychology, 139(4), 244-259. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2012.697932

Swami, V., Voracek, M., Stieger, S., Tran, U. S., & Furnham, A. (2014). Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition, 133(3), 572-585. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.006

Swami, V., Weis, L., Lay, A., Barron, D., & Furnham, A. (2016). Associations between belief in conspiracy theories and the maladaptive personality traits of the personality inventory for DSM-5. Psychiatry Research, 236, 86-90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.12.027

Trifonova, T. (2017). Agência nos thrillers cinematográficos de conspiração. Rumores, 11(22), 59-88.

Van Prooijen, J. W., & Acker, M. (2015). The influence of control on belief in conspiracy theories: Conceptual and applied extensions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29(5), 753-761. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.3161

Van Prooijen, J. W., & Jostmann, N. B. (2013). Belief in conspiracy theories: The influence of uncertainty and perceived morality. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(1), 109-115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.1922

Van Prooijen, J. W., Krouwel, A. P., & Pollet, T. V. (2015). Political extremism predicts belief in conspiracy theories. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6(5), 570-578. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550614567356

Wagner-Egger, P., Bangerter, A., Gilles, I., Green, E., Rigaud, D., Krings, F., ... & Clémence, A. (2011). Lay perceptions of collectives at the outbreak of the H1N1 epidemic: Heroes, villains and victims. Public Understanding of Science, 20(4), 461-476. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963662510393605

Published

2023-02-14

How to Cite

REZENDE, A. T., SILVA, F. M. de S. M. da, RIBEIRO, M. G. C., LOURETO, G. D. L., SILVA NETA, O. F. da, & GOUVEIA, V. V. (2023). Conspiracy theories: Meanings in the Brazilian context. Psychological Studies, 36. Retrieved from https://periodicos.puc-campinas.edu.br/estpsi/article/view/7420

Issue

Section

SOCIAL AND ORGANIZATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY