Teorias da conspiração: signifi cados em contexto brasileiro
Palavras-chave:
Crenças, Signifi cado, TeoriasResumo
O presente estudo teve como objetivo conhecer os signifi cados atribuídos às teorias da conspiração em amostra brasileira. Participaram 383 estudantes universitários (54,6% mulheres; Midade = 21,9) de uma instituição pública de João Pessoa, Paraíba, que responderam a perguntas demográfi cas e a duas questões abertas (O que você entende por teorias da conspiração? O que lhe vem à cabeça quando você pensa em teorias da conspiração?). A partir da análise de Classifi cação Hierárquica Descendente, identifi caram-se cinco classes: (1) teorias sem embasamento científi co, (2) manipulação de grupos secretos, (3) explicação da realidade social, (4) contestação de fatos sociais e (5) controle de informação. No geral, os signifi cados atribuídos às teorias da conspiração foram atrelados à explicação de acontecimentos da realidade social e à existência de grupos secretos que controlam eventos importantes. Tais resultados foram discutidos à luz de estudos sobre as teorias da conspiração em Psicologia, e revelaram congruência com a concepção que se construiu desse construto.
Downloads
Referências
Aaronovitch, D., & Langton, J. (2010). Voodoo histories: The role of the conspiracy theory in shaping modern history. New York: Riverhead Books.
Barron, D., Morgan, K., Towell, T., Altemeyer, B., & Swami, V. (2014). Associations between schizotypy and belief in conspiracist ideation. Personality and Individual Differences, 70, 156-159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.04
Bessi, A., Coletto, M., Davidescu, G. A., Scala, A., Caldarelli, G., & Quattrociocchi, W. (2015). Science vs conspiracy: Collective narratives in the age of misinformation. PLoS One, 10(2), 1-17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.011809
Bogart, L. M., & Thorburn, S. (2006). Relationship of African Americans’ sociodemographic characteristics to belief in conspiracies about HIV/AIDS and birth control. Journal of the National Medical Association, 98(7), 1144-1150.
Brotherton, R., & Eser, S. (2015). Bored to fears: Boredom proneness, paranoia, and conspiracy theories. Personality and Individual Differences, 80, 1-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.01
Byford, J. (2011). Conspiracy theories: A critical introduction. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
Byford, J. (2014). Beyond belief: The social psychology of conspiracy theories and the study of ideology. In C. Antaki & S. Condor (Eds.), Rhetoric, ideology and social psychology: Essays in honour of Michael Billig (pp. 83-94). London:
Routledge.
Camargo, B. V. (2005). ALCESTE: um programa informático de análise quantitativa de dados textuais. Perspectivas Teórico-Metodológicas em Representações Sociais, 1(3), 511-539.
Camargo, B. V., & Justo, A. M. (2013). IRAMUTEQ: um software gratuito para análise de dados textuais. Temas em Psicologia, 21(2), 513-518. http://dx.doi.org/10.9788/TP2013.2-16
Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2008). The hidden impact of conspiracy theories: Perceived and actual influence of theories surrounding the death of Princess Diana. The Journal of Social Psychology, 148(2), 210-222..
Drinkwater, K., Dagnall, N., & Parker, A. (2012). Reality testing, conspiracy theories, and paranormal beliefs. The Journal of Parapsychology, 76(1), 57-68.
Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 15(4), 731-742. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3791630
Goertzel, T. (2010). Conspiracy theories in science. EMBO Reports, 11(7), 493-499. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/embor.2010.8
Gouveia, V. V. (2013). Teoria funcionalista dos valores humanos: fundamentos, aplicações e perspectivas. São Paulo: Caso do Psicólogo.
Hofstadter, R. (1966). The paranoid style in American politics. In R. Hofstader (Ed.), The paranoid style in American politics and other essays (pp. 3-40). New York: Knopf.
Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (2014a). The effects of anti-vaccine conspiracy theories on vaccination intentions. PLoS One, 9(2), 898-906. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0089177
Jolley, D., & Douglas, K. M. (2014b). The social consequences of conspiracism: Exposure to conspiracy theories decreases intentions to engage in politics and to reduce one’s carbon footprint. British Journal of Psychology, 105(1), 35-56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12018
Jolley, D., Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2017). Blaming a few bad apples to save a threatened barrel: The systemjustifying function of conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 39(2), 465-478. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/pops.12404
Knight, P. (2008). Outrageous conspiracy theories: Popular and official responses to 9/11 in Germany and the United States. New German Critique, 35(1), 165-193. http://dx.doi.org/10.1215/0094033X-2007-024
Kruglanski, A. W. (1987). Blame-placing schemata and attributional research. In C. F. Graumann & S. Moscovici (Eds.), Changing conceptions of conspiracy (pp. 219-229). New York: Springer.
Leman, P. J., & Cinnirella, M. (2013). Beliefs in conspiracy theories and the need for cognitive closure. Frontiers in Psychology, 378(4), 1-10. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00378
Lobato, E., Mendoza, J., Sims, V., & Chin, M. (2014). Examining the relationship between conspiracy theories, paranormal beliefs, and pseudoscience acceptance among a university population. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(5), 617-625. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.3042
Moulding, R., Nix-Carnell, S., Schnabel, A., Nedeljkovic, M., Burnside, E. E., Lentini, A. F., & Mehzabin, N. (2016). Better the devil you know than a world you don’t? Intolerance of uncertainty and worldview explanations for belief in conspiracy theories. Personality and Individual Differences, 98, 345-354. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.060
Nicolas, L. (2017). As teorias da conspiração como espelho do século: entre a retórica, a sociologia e a história das ideias. Revista Eletrônica de Estudos Integrados em Discurso e Argumentação, 12, 255-279. Recuperado em outubro 23, 2017, de http://periodicos.uesc.br/index.php/eidea/article/viewFile/1325/1087
Oliver, J. E., & Wood, T. J. (2014). Conspiracy theories and the paranoid style(s) of mass opinion. American Journal of Political Science, 58(4), 952-966. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12084
R Development Core Team (2012). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Ratinaud, P. (2009). IRAMUTEQ: Interface de R pour les analyses multidimensionnelles de textes et de questionnaires [Computer software]. Retrieved October 23, 2017, from http://www. iramuteq. org
Ratinaud, P., & Marchand, P. (2012). Application de la méthode ALCESTE à de “gros” corpus et stabilité des “mondes lexicaux”: Analyse du “Cable-Gate” avec IraMuTeQ. In Actes des 11eme Journées internationales d’Analyse Statistique des Données Textuelles (pp. 835-844). Récupéré dans Octobre 23, 2017, from http://lexicometrica.univparis3.fr/jadt/jadt2012/Communications/Ratinaud,%20Pierre%20et%20al.%20%20Application%20de%20la%20methode%20Alceste.pdf
Sapountzis, A., & Condor, S. (2013). Conspiracy accounts as intergroup theories: Challenging dominant understandings of social power and political legitimacy. Political Psychology, 34(5), 731-752. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/pops.1201
Swami, V., & Coles, R. (2010). The truth is out there: Belief in conspiracy theories. The Psychologist, 23(7), 560-563.
Swami, V., & Furnham, A. (2012). Examining conspiracist beliefs about the disappearance of Amelia Earhart. The Journal of General Psychology, 139(4), 244-259. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2012.697932
Swami, V., Voracek, M., Stieger, S., Tran, U. S., & Furnham, A. (2014). Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition, 133(3), 572-585. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.006
Swami, V., Weis, L., Lay, A., Barron, D., & Furnham, A. (2016). Associations between belief in conspiracy theories and the maladaptive personality traits of the personality inventory for DSM-5. Psychiatry Research, 236, 86-90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.12.027
Trifonova, T. (2017). Agência nos thrillers cinematográficos de conspiração. Rumores, 11(22), 59-88.
Van Prooijen, J. W., & Acker, M. (2015). The influence of control on belief in conspiracy theories: Conceptual and applied extensions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29(5), 753-761. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.3161
Van Prooijen, J. W., & Jostmann, N. B. (2013). Belief in conspiracy theories: The influence of uncertainty and perceived morality. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(1), 109-115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.1922
Van Prooijen, J. W., Krouwel, A. P., & Pollet, T. V. (2015). Political extremism predicts belief in conspiracy theories. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6(5), 570-578. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550614567356
Wagner-Egger, P., Bangerter, A., Gilles, I., Green, E., Rigaud, D., Krings, F., ... & Clémence, A. (2011). Lay perceptions of collectives at the outbreak of the H1N1 epidemic: Heroes, villains and victims. Public Understanding of Science, 20(4), 461-476. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963662510393605
Downloads
Publicado
Como Citar
Edição
Seção
Licença
Copyright (c) 2023 Alessandro Teixeira REZENDE, Flávia Marcelly de Sousa Mendes da SILVA, Maria Gabriela Costa RIBEIRO, Gleidson Diego Lopes LOURETO, Olindina Fernandes da SILVA NETA, Valdiney Veloso GOUVEIA
Este trabalho está licenciado sob uma licença Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.