Treinamento de incompatibilidade espacial pode prevenir a ocorrência do efeito Simon ampliado em idosos
Palavras-chave:
Envelhecimento, Controle inibitório, Percepção espacial, Transferência de aprendizagemResumo
O efeito Simon é tipicamente maior em pessoas idosas do que em adultos jovens, talvez devido ao declínio de capacidades inibitórias em função da idade. Participaram deste estudo 32 pessoas idosas e destras (5 homens e 27 mulheres; média de 67,1 anos ± 5,5). Para rastreio, foram usados o Mini Exame do Estado Mental e a escala de depressão do Center for Epidemiological Studies. Metade dos participantes praticou a tarefa de incompatibilidade espacial antes da tarefa de Simon e o outro grupo foi testado apenas na tarefa de Simon. A análise de variância, considerando a prática prévia (incompatível ou sem prática) e correspondência (correspondente, não correspondente), indicou uma interação dupla (F = 7,07; p = 0,012; ηp2 = 0,191), demonstrando que o efeito Simon foi eliminado no grupo de prática incompatível, mas permaneceu ampliado no grupo sem prática anterior. Esses resultados indicaram que os processos cognitivos necessários para transferir a aprendizagem estão preservados em idosos, sendo potencialmente úteis para influenciar as capacidades inibitórias espaciais.
Downloads
Referências
Aisenberg, D., Sapir, A., Close, A., Henik, A., & d’Avossa, G. (2018). Right anterior cerebellum BOLD responses reflect age related changes in Simon task sequential effects. Neuropsychologia, 109, 155-164. https://doi.org/10.1093/ geronb/gbaa184
Ambrosecchia, M., Marino, B. F., Gawryszewski, L. G., & Riggio, L. (2015). Spatial stimulus-response compatibility and affordance effects are not ruled by the same mechanisms. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 283. https://doi. org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00283
Bae, G. Y., Cho, Y. S., & Proctor, R. W. (2009). Transfer of orthogonal stimulus-response mappings to an orthogonal Simon task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(4), 746-765. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210802303883
Batistoni, S. S. T., Néri, A. L., & Cupertino, A. P. (2010). Validade e confiabilidade da versão Brasileira da Center for Epidemiological Scale-Depression (CES-D) em idosos Brasileiros. Psico-USF, 15(1), 13-22. https://doi.org/10.1590/ S1413-82712010000100003
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290
Buchanan, J. A., Johnson, E., Kennedy, J., Stypulkowski, K., & Jensen, N. (2019). The effects of cognitive training program for cognitively impaired older adults: a pilot randomized control trial. International Journal of Aging Research, 2(2), 30-30. https://doi.org/10.28933/ijoar-2019-03-0705
Butler, M., McCreedy, E., Nelson, V. A., Desai, P., Ratner, E., Fink, H. A., Hemmy, L. S., McCarten, J. R., Barclay, T. R., Brasure, M., Davila, H., & Kane, R. L. (2018). Does cognitive training prevent cognitive decline? A systematic review. Annals of Internal Medicine, 168(1), 63-68. https://doi.org/10.7326/M17-1531
Cabeza, R., Nyberg, L., & Park, D. C. (2016). Cognitive neuroscience of aging: linking cognitive and cerebral aging. Oxford University Press.
Cespón, J., Hommel, B., Korsch, M., & Galashan, D. (2020). The neurocognitive underpinnings of the Simon effect: an integrative review of current research. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 20, 1-40. https://doi. org/10.3758/s13415-020-00836-y
Cohen, J. (2013). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Routledge.
Conde, E. F. Q., Fraga Filho, R. S., Lameira, A. P., Riggio, L., & Gawryszewski, L. G. (2017). Stimulus-response dimensional overlap elicits a decreasing Simon effect along the vertical dimension. Estudos de Psicologia, 22(4), 358-365. http:// dx.doi.org/10.22491/1678-4669.20170037
Conde, E. F. Q., Fraga-Filho, R. S., Lameira, A. P., Mograbi, D. C., Riggio, L., & Gawryszewski, L. G. (2015). Influence of short incompatible practice on the Simon effect: transfer along the vertical dimension and across vertical and horizontal dimensions. Experimental Brain Research, 233(11), 3313-3321. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4399-1
Creekmur, B., & Vu, K. P. (2011). Effect of prior practice on the stimulus-response compatibility effect in a mixed mapping environment. The American Journal of Psychology, 125(3), 335-349. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.125.3.0335
D’Ascenzo, S., Lugli, L., Nicoletti, R., & Umiltà, C. (2021). Practice effects vs. transfer effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 85, 1955-1969. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01386-1
Espírito-Santo, H., Pires, C. F., Garcia, I. Q., Daniel, F., Silva, A. G. D., & Fazio, R. L. (2017). Preliminary validation of the Portuguese Edinburgh Handedness Inventory in an adult sample. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 24(3), 275-287.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191. https://doi.org/10.3758/ BF03193146
Fjell, A. M., Westlye, L. T., Amlien, I., Espeseth, T., Reinvang, I., Raz, N., Agartz, I., Salat, D. H., Greve, D. N., Fischl, B., Dale, A. M., & Walhovd, K. B. (2009). High consistency of regional cortical thinning in aging across multiple samples. Cerebral Cortex, 19(9), 2001-2012. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn232
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189-198. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022- 3956(75)90026-6
Fuster, J. M. (2009). Cortex and memory: emergence of a new paradigm. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(11), 2047-2072. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21280
Grabbe, J. W., & Allen, P. A. (2012). Cross-task compatibility and age-related dual-task performance. Experimental Aging Research, 38(5), 469-487. https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2012.726154
Grady, C. (2012). The cognitive neuroscience of ageing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(7), 491-505. https://doi. org/10.1038/nrn3256
Hill, N. T., Mowszowski, L., Naismith, S. L., Chadwick, V. L., Valenzuela, M., & Lampit, A. (2016). Computerized cognitive training in older adults with mild cognitive impairment or dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 174(4), 329-340. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16030360
Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 849-878.
Hommel, B. (2011). The Simon effect as tool and heuristic. Acta Psychologica, 136(2), 189-202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. actpsy.2010.04.011
Hommel, B., & Lippa, Y. (1995). SR compatibility effects due to context-dependent spatial stimulus coding. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2(3), 370-374.
Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. L. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychological Research, 68(1), 1-17. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210974
Iani, C., Rubichi, S., Gherri, E., & Nicoletti, R. (2009). Co-occurrence of sequential and practice effects in the Simon task: Evidence for two independent mechanisms affecting response selection. Memory & Cognition, 37(3), 358-367. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.37.3.358
Ivanoff, J. (2003). On spatial response code activation in a Simon task. Acta Psychologica, 112(2), 157-179. https://doi. org/10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00081-1
Kattenstroth, J. C., Kolankowska, I., Kalisch, T., & Dinse, H. R. (2010). Superior sensory, motor, and cognitive performance in elderly individuals with multi-year dancing activities. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 2(31), 1-9. https://doi. org/10.3389/fnagi.2010.00031
Kawai, N., Kubo-Kawai, N., Kubo, K., Terazawa, T., & Masataka, N. (2012). Distinct aging effects for two types of inhibition in older adults: a near-infrared spectroscopy study on the Simon task and the flanker task. Neuroreport, 23(14), 819-824. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283578032
Krampe, R. T. (2002). Aging, expertise and fine motor movement. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(7), 769-776. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00064-7
Kubo-Kawai, N., & Kawai, N. (2010). Elimination of the enhanced Simon effect for older adults in a three-choice situation: ageing and the Simon effect in a go/no-go Simon task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(3), 452- 64. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210902990829
Liu, X., Banich, M. T., Jacobson, B. L., & Tanabe, J. L. (2004). Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and spatial Stroop task as assessed by event-related fMRI. Neuroimage, 22(3), 1097-1106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.02.033
Luo, C., & Proctor, R. W. (2019). How different direct association routes influence the indirect route in the same Simon-like task. Psychological Research, 83(8), 1733-1748. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1024-5
Lupien, S. J., & Wan, N. (2004). Successful ageing: from cell to self. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 359(1449), 1413-1426. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2004.1516
MacPherson, S. E., Phillips, L. H., & Della Sala, S. (2002). Age, executive function and social decision making: a dorsolateral prefrontal theory of cognitive aging. Psychology and Aging, 17(4), 598. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.598
Marini, M., Iani, C., Nicoletti, R., & Rubichi, S. (2011). Between-task transfer of learning from spatial compatibility to a color Stroop task. Experimental Psychology, 58(6), 473-479. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000115
McLaughlin, S. J., Jette, A. M., & Connell, C. M. (2012). An examination of healthy aging across a conceptual continuum: Prevalence estimates, demographic patterns, and validity. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biomedical Sciences and Medical Sciences, 67(7), 783-789. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glr234
Melara, R. D., Wang, H., Vu, K. P. L., & Proctor, R. W. (2008). Attentional origins of the Simon effect: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Brain Research, 1215, 147-159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2008.03.026
Melo, D. M. D., & Barbosa, A. J. G. (2015). Use of the Mini-Mental State Examination in research on the elderly in Brazil: a systematic review. Ciência & Saúde Coletiva, 20(12), 3865-3876. https://doi.org/10.1590/1413- 812320152012.06032015
Melo, D. M. D., Barbosa, A. J. G., Castro, N. R. D., & Neri, A. L. (2020). Mini-Mental State Examination in Brazil: an item response theory analysis. Paidéia, 30, e3014. https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-4327e3014
Miles, J. D., & Vu, K. P. L. (2021). Individual Response-Effect Congruencies Modulate Subsequent Stimulus - Response Compatibility Effects. The American Journal of Psychology, 134(1), 31-43. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.134.1.0031
Millner, A. J., Jaroszewski, A. C., Chamarthi, H., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2012). Behavioral and electrophysiological correlates of training-induced cognitive control improvements. NeuroImage, 63(2), 742-753. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. neuroimage.2012.07.032
Morrison, J. H., & Baxter, M. G. (2012). The ageing cortical synapse: hallmarks and implications for cognitive decline. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(4), 240. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3200
Nagy, B., Czigler, I., File, D., & Gaál, Z. A. (2020). Can irrelevant but salient visual cues compensate for the age-related decline in cognitive conflict resolution? An ERP study. Plos One, 15(5), e0233496. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal. pone.0233496
Nissim, N. R., O’Shea, A. M., Bryant, V., Porges, E. C., Cohen, R., & Woods, A. J. (2017). Frontal structural neural correlates of working memory performance in older adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 8, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.3389/ fnagi.2016.00328
Nyberg, L., & Pudas, S. (2019). Successful memory aging. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 219-243. https://doi. org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103052
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburg Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97-113. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
Opie, G. M., & Semmler, J. G. (2020). Characterising the influence of cerebellum on the neuroplastic modulation of intracortical motor circuits. Plos One, 15(7), e0236005. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0236005
Ottoboni, G., Iani, C., Tessari, A., & Rubichi, S. (2013). Modulation of the affordance effect through transfer of learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(12), 2295-2302. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1080/174702 18.2013.863370
Park, D. C., Polk, T. A., Mikels, J. A., Taylor, S. F., & Marshuetz, C. (2001). Cerebral aging: integration of brain and behavioral models of cognitive function. Dialogues Clinical Neuroscience, 3(3), 151-65. https://doi.org/10.31887/ dcns.2001.3.3/dcpark
Persson, J., Nyberg, L., Lind, J., Larsson, A., Nilsson, L. G., Ingvar, M., & Buckner, R. L. (2006). Structure - function correlates of cognitive decline in aging. Cerebral Cortex, 16(7), 907-915. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhj036
Peterson, B. S., Kane, M. J., Alexander, G. M., Lacadie, C., Skudlarski, P., Leung, H. C., May, J., & Gore, J. C. (2002). An event-related functional MRI study comparing interference effects in the Simon and Stroop tasks. Cognitive Brain Research, 13(3), 427-440. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00054-x
Pick, D. F., & Proctor, R. W. (1999). Age differences in the effects of irrelevant location information. In M. Scerbo & M. W. Mouloua (Eds.), Automation technology and human performance (pp. 258-261). Erlbaum.
Proctor, R. W., & Vu, K. P. L. (2006). Stimulus-response compatibility principles: data, theory, and applications. Taylor and Francis. https://doi.org/10.1201/9780203022795
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385-401. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F014662167700100306
Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., & Sheliga, B.M. (1994). Space and selective attention. In C. Umiltà & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance (pp. 231-265). MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00240962
Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., & Umiltá, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia, 25(1), 31-40. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028- 3932(87)90041-8
Scrivano, R. M., & Kieffaber, P. D. (2021). Behavioral and electrophysiological correlates of Simon and flanker conflict interference in younger and older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 29(2), 1-31. https://doi.org/10.1 080/13825585.2021.1874278
Soetens E., Maetens K., & Zeischka, P. (2010). Practice-induced and sequential modulations of the Simon effect. Atten Percept Psychophys, 72(4), 895-911. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00240962
Spierer, L., Chavan, C. F., & Manuel, A. L. (2013). Training induced behavioral and brain plasticity in inhibitory control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 427. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00427
Stoet, G. (2017). Sex differences in the Simon task help to interpret sex differences in selective attention. Psychological Research, 81(3), 571-581. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0763-4
Tagliabue, M., Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (2002). Cross-modal re-mapping influences the Simon effect. Memory and Cognition, 30, 18-23. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195261
Tagliabue, M., Zorzi, M., Umiltà, C., & Bassignani, F. (2000). The role of long-term-memory and short-term-memory links in the Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(2), 648-670. https:// psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0096-1523.26.2.648
Van der Lubbe, R. H., & Verleger, R. (2002). Aging and the Simon task. Psychophysiology, 39(01), 100-110. https://doi. org/10.1111/1469-8986.3910100
Verghese, A., Mattingley, J. B., Palmer, P. E., & Dux, P. E. (2018). From eyes to hands: Transfer of learning in the Simon task across motor effectors. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 80(1), 193-210. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.3758/ s13414-017-1427-1
Vermeij, A., Claassen, J. A., Dautzenberg, P. L., & Kessels, R. P. (2016). Transfer and maintenance effects of online working-memory training in normal ageing and mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 26(5-6), 783-809. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2015.1048694
Vu, K. P. L. (2007). Influences on the Simon effect of prior practice with spatially incompatible mappings: transfer within and between horizontal and vertical dimensions. Memory & Cognition, 35(6), 1463-1471. https://doi.org/10.3758/ BF03193616
Vu, K. P. L., & Proctor, R. W. (2008). Age differences in response selection for pure and mixed stimulus-response mappings and tasks. Acta Psychologica, 129(1), 49-60. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.actpsy.2008.04.006
Wang, L., & Weekes, B. (2014). Neural correlates of the Simon effect modulated by practice with spatial mapping. Neuropsychologia, 63, 72-84. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.019
Wang, M., Gamo, N. J., Yang, Y., Jin, L. E., Wang, X. J., Laubach, M., Mazer, J. A., Lee, D., & Arnsten, A. F. (2011). Neuronal basis of age-related working memory decline. Nature, 476(7359), e210. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature10243
Ward, E., Berry, C., & Shanks, D. (2013). Age effects on explicit and implicit memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, e639. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00639
Wollesen, B., Wildbredt, A., van Schooten, K. S., Lim, M. L., & Delbaere, K. (2020). The effects of cognitive-motor training interventions on executive functions in older people: a systematic review and meta-analysis. European Review of Aging and Physical Activity, 17(1), 1-22. https://doi.org/10.1186/s11556-020-00240-y
Yamaguchi, M., & Proctor, R. W. (2009). Transfer of learning in choice reactions: contributions of specific and general components of manual responses. Acta Psychologica, 130(1), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.09.008
Zhong, Q., Xiong, A., Vu, K. P. L., & Proctor, R. W. (2018). Vertically arrayed stimuli and responses: transfer of incompatible spatial mapping to Simon task occurs regardless of response-device orientation. Experimental Brain Research, 236(1), 175-185. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-020-01998-0
Downloads
Publicado
Como Citar
Edição
Seção
Licença
Copyright (c) 2023 Taciana Elaine de Moura DIAS, Fabíola Freire Lauria CAVALCANTI, Walter MACHADO-PINHEIRO, André dos Santos COSTA, Erick Francisco Quintas CONDE
Este trabalho está licenciado sob uma licença Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.